When the Tongue Slips, the Eyes Have It

*********************************
There is now a CONTENT FREEZE for Mercury while we switch to a new platform. It began on Friday, March 10 at 6pm and will end on Wednesday, March 15 at noon. No new content can be created during this time, but all material in the system as of the beginning of the freeze will be migrated to the new platform, including users and groups. Functionally the new site is identical to the old one. webteam@gatech.edu
*********************************

Contact

Georgia Tech Media Relations
Laura Diamond
laura.diamond@comm.gatech.edu
404-894-6016
Jason Maderer
maderer@gatech.edu
404-660-2926

Sidebar Content
No sidebar content submitted.
Summaries

Summary Sentence:

Eyes provide clues when speakers make mistakes.

Full Summary:

When people mislabel objects, they often blame themselves for rushing their words or not paying attention. But research at Georgia Tech suggests the mistakes may have less to do with concentration than previously thought.

Media

How is it that we can look at a door and accidentally call it a window or call a shovel a rake? When people mislabel objects, they often blame themselves for rushing their words or not paying attention. But research at the Georgia Institute of Technology, published in the December issue of Psychological Science, suggests the mistakes may have less to do with concentration than previously thought. The findings provide an insight into how the brain organizes speech and suggests that when the tongue slips, the eyes may be the best window into a speaker's intent.

"People typically look at objects before naming them, it's part of the way they plan the words they are going to say, said Zenzi Griffin, assistant professor of psychology at Georgia Tech. "So, if people are rushing or being inattentive you might expect if they made an error that they spent less time looking at the object. But I found almost no difference in the amount of time people spent looking at an object when they made an error compared to when they didn't. In fact, people who made an error spent slightly more time looking at the object."

In the study, Griffin asked participants to name two or three line-drawn objects or describe the action in a scene, while she tracked their eye movements using video cameras outfitted with special software. She identified 41 full or partial speech errors uttered by 33 participants during eye-tracking experiments.

The results, said Griffin, show that at some level people know what they meant to say and that looking at the object doesn't help to ensure that they will name it correctly. They also suggest that when a person makes a speech error, knowing what they are looking at may be more informative of their intentions than the words they say.

That may be useful to designers of speech recognition software, said Griffin. "Gaze can potentially provide clues to what uncertain words are - at least when people are talking about things in their immediate environment, like in a cockpit or an automobile," she said. "Gaze can also help to disambiguate which object you are referring to, so if you say 'Open the door,' the software could know to which door you are referring."

Media Contact: David Terraso, 404-385-2966, david.terraso@icpa.gatech.edu

Technical Contact: Zenzi Griffin, 404-894-6771, zenzi.griffin@psych.gatech.edu

Additional Information

Groups

News Room

Categories
No categories were selected.
Related Core Research Areas
No core research areas were selected.
Newsroom Topics
No newsroom topics were selected.
Keywords
No keywords were submitted.
Status
  • Created By: David Terraso
  • Workflow Status: Published
  • Created On: Jan 19, 2005 - 8:00pm
  • Last Updated: Oct 7, 2016 - 11:02pm